Effectiveness of spatial training in elementary and secondary school: everyone learns

Authors

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.5944/educxx1.30100

Keywords:

mental rotation, training, gender differences, achievement gains, educational strategies

Agencies:

Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades

Abstract

Visuospatial processing is key to achieve optimal performance in academic activities, among others. In the field of spatial cognition, it has been found that practice with spatial tasks can reduce the gender gap in this type of reasoning. However, an increase in spatial scores does not always compensate the differences that exist between participants with greater and lesser spatial abilities. According to previous studies on individual differences and malleability in spatial cognition, comparable studies are needed in children and adolescents using the same evaluation and training method. 39 students of Primary Education (study 1: 17 boys and 22 girls) and 21 of Secondary Education (study 2: 11 boys and 10 girls) participated in this study. The evolution profile through three sessions in a mental rotation (RM) training was analyzed, as well as the degree of improvement produced based on their initial spatial performance, analyzing the gender factor. In both groups, a spatial intervention (Mental Rotation Training Program) was applied during three consecutive sessions with an average duration of 35 minutes per session. For both age groups, participants with a low spatial level benefited at a similar rate to those participants with more spatial resources. This result was replicated for both sexes. This research will serve as a starting point to promote and implement adaptive and personalized training, and thus be able to help those who have fewer spatial skills. These types of interventions would become more effective and could maximize educational potential in the most disadvantaged groups.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

References

Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20(5-6), 327–344. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00287729

Baniqued, P. L., Kranz, M. B., Voss, M. W., Lee, H., Cosman, J. D., Severson, J., & Kramer, A. F. (2014). Cognitive training with casual video games: points to consider. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1010. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01010

Bergner, S., & Neubauer, A. C. (2011). Sex and training differences in mental rotation: A behavioral and neurophysiological comparison of gifted achievers, gifted underachievers and average intelligent achievers. High Ability Studies, 22(2), 155–177. https://doi.org/10.1080/13598139.2011.628849

Cherney, I. D., Bersted, K., & Smetter, J. (2014). Training spatial skills in men and women. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 119(1), 82–99. https://doi.org/10.2466/23.25.PMS.119c12z0

Contreras, M. J., Escrig, R., Prieto, G., & Elosúa, M. R. (2018). Spatial Visualization ability improves with and without studying Technical Drawing. Cognitive Processing, 19(3), 387–397. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-018-0859-4

David, L. T. (2012). Training effects on mental rotation, spatial orientation and spatial visualisation depending on the initial level of spatial abilities. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 33, 328–332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.01.137

De Lisi, R., & Wolford, J. (2002). Improving children’s mental rotation accuracy with computer game playing. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163(3), 272–283. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221320209598683

Ehrlich, S. B., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The importance of gesture in children’s spatial reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1259–1268. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1259

Fernández-Méndez, L. M., Contreras, M. J., & Elosúa, M. R. (2020). Developmental differences between 1st and 3rd year of early childhood education (preschool) in mental rotation and its training. Psychological Research, 84(4), 1056-1064. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1104-6

García‐Madruga, J. A., Elosúa, M. R., Gil, L., Gómez‐Veiga, I., Vila, J. Ó., Orjales, I.,..., & Duque, G. (2013). Reading comprehension and working memory's executive processes: An intervention study in primary school students. Reading Research Quarterly, 48(2), 155–174. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.44

Hawes, Z., Moss, J., Caswell, B., Naqvi, S., & MacKinnon, S. (2017). Enhancing children's spatial and numerical skills through a dynamic spatial approach to early geometry instruction: effects of a 32-week intervention. Cognition and Instruction, 35(3) 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2017.1323902

Humphreys, L. G., Lubinski, D., & Yao, G. (1993). Utility of predicting group membership and the role of spatial visualization in becoming an engineer, physical scientist, or artist. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(2), 250–261. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.78.2.250.

Karbach, J., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2015). Adaptive working-memory training benefits reading, but not mathematics in middle childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 21(3), 285–301. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2014.899336

Linn, M. C., & Hyde, J. S. (1989). Gender, mathematics, and science. Educational Researcher, 18(8), 17–27. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X018008017

Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56(6), 1479–1498. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1985.tb00213.x

Lohman, D. F. (1996). Spatial ability and g. In I. Dennis & P. Tapsfield (Eds.), Human abilities: Their nature and measurement (pp. 97–116). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Lowrie, T., Logan, T., & Ramful, A. (2017). Visuospatial training improves elementary students’ mathematics performance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(2), 170–186. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12142

Neubauer, A. C., Bergner, S., & Schatz, M. (2010). Two-vs. three-dimensional presentation of mental rotation tasks: Sex differences and effects of training on performance and brain activation. Intelligence, 38(5), 529–539. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2010.06.001

Rafi, A., Samsudin, K. A., & Said, C. S. (2008). Training in spatial visualization: The effects of training method and gender. Educational Technology & Society, 11(3), 127–140.

Raven, J., Court, J. H. & Raven, J. C. (1996). Standard Progressive Matrices. Psychologists Press.

Rodán, A., Contreras, M. J., Elosúa, M. R., & Gimeno, P. (2016). Experimental but not sex differences of a mental rotation training program on adolescents. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1050. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01050

Rodán, A., Gimeno, P., Elosúa, M. R., Montoro, P. R., & Contreras, M. J. (2019). Boys and girls gain in spatial, but not in mathematical ability after mental rotation training in primary education. Learning and Individual Differences, 70, 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2019.01.001

Samsudin, K., Rafi, A., & Hanif, A. S. (2011). Training in mental rotation and spatial visualization and its impact on orthographic drawing performance. Journal of Educational Technology & Society, 14(1), 179–186.

Santamaría, P., Arribas, D., Pereña, J., & Seisdedos, N. (2005). EFAI, Evaluación Factorial de las Aptitudes Intelectuales. Departamento I+D TEA Ediciones.

Sanz de Acedo Lizarraga, M. L., & García Ganuza, J. M. (2003). Improvement of mental rotation in girls and boys. Sex Roles, 49(5–6), 277–286. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024656408294

Seisdedos, N. (1995). Manual de las Matrices Progresivas de Raven. Escalas CPM, SPM y APM. TEA Ediciones.

Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–703. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.171.3972.701

Sims, V. K., & Mayer, R. E. (2002). Domain specificity of spatial expertise: The case of video game players. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 16(1), 97–115. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.759

Terlecki, M. S., Newcombe, N. S., & Little, M. (2008). Durable and generalized effects of spatial experience on mental rotation: Gender differences in growth patterns. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 22(7), 996–1013. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1420

Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 352–402. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028446

Voyer, D., Rodgers, M. A., & McCormick, P. A. (2004). Timing conditions and the magnitude of gender differences on the Mental Rotations Test. Memory & Cognition, 32(1), 72–82. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195821

Voyer, D. & Jansen, P. (2016). Sex differences in chronometric mental rotation with human bodies. Psychological Research, 80(6), 974–984. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0701-x

Wiedenbauer, G., & Jansen-Osmann, P. (2007). Mental rotation ability of children with spina bifida: what influence does manual rotation training have? Developmental Neuropsychology, 32(3), 809–824. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640701539626

Wiedenbauer, G., & Jansen-Osmann, P. (2008). Manual training of mental rotation in children. Learning and Instruction, 18(1), 30–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.09.009

Published

2022-01-03

Issue

Section

Estudios